婵☆偓绲鹃悧鐘诲Υ閿燂拷
闂佸搫鐗忛崰搴ㄥ垂閿燂拷: 閻庣敻鍋婇崰鏇熺┍婵犲洤妫橀柛銉㈡櫇瑜帮拷 闂侀潻璐熼崝搴ㄥ吹鎼淬垻鈻曢柨鏂垮⒔濞硷拷 闁荤姍鍐仾闁哄鍓濈粙澶屸偓锝庡幗缁侊拷 闂佺ǹ顑冮崕閬嶅箖瀹ュ绾ч柛灞炬皑缁狅拷 闂佺缈伴崕閬嶅垂閺冨牊鐒诲璺虹灱濡烇拷 闂佺硶鏅炲▍锝夈€侀崨鏉戠閻犲洤妯婇崝锟� 婵炴垶鎸诲ḿ娆戣姳閵夆晛绀岄悹鍥ф▕閸旓拷 闂佽偐鐡旈崹浼搭敆閿燂拷 闁诲孩鍐婚梽鍕叏閿燂拷 婵炴垶鎼╅崢鐓幟瑰Δ鍛閻犲洤妯婇崝锟� 闂佸憡顨忛崑鍡涘极閹捐绠戦柡鍕禋閸燂拷
婵烇絽娲︾换鍌涚閿燂拷: 闂佸搫鍊烽崡鍐差渻閿燂拷 闁荤姴娲ょ€氼垶顢欓敓锟� 闁荤喐鐟ュΛ娑㈠磻閿燂拷 闁汇埄鍨遍悺鏇㈡儊閿燂拷 闂佹眹鍊曟晶钘夘潖閿燂拷 闂佹眹鍎卞ú銊︽叏閿燂拷 闂佺ǹ绻楀畷鐢稿极閿燂拷 闂佺儵鍋撻崝蹇撶暤閿燂拷 闂佺缈伴崐妤呭箖閿燂拷 闂佽鍏欓崕鏌ュ疾閿燂拷 闂佺ǹ顑冮崕鎾閿燂拷 缂傚倸娲ゆ鎼侇敊閿燂拷 婵炴垶鎸堕崐鏍焵椤掑﹥瀚� 闂佺厧寮惰ぐ鍐礉閿燂拷 闂佹椿婢侀幏锟� 婵犻潧鍋婇幏锟� 闂佺厧搴滈幏锟� 闁诲繐楠忛幏锟� 闂佹悶鍎茬粙鎴︻敆閿燂拷 闁荤姴娲╁畷鍨櫠閿燂拷 闂佸搫娲﹀ḿ娆撍囬敓锟�
婵炴垶鎼╅崢鑲╀焊閿燂拷: 闁汇埄鍨遍悺鏇㈡儊閿燂拷 闂佽桨鐒﹂悷锕€岣块敓锟� 闂佽鍓氱换鍡涘焵椤掑﹥瀚� 婵炴垶鎼╅崣鈧€规搫鎷� 闂佸憡鐗炲畷鐢稿箖閿燂拷 婵炴垶鎸诲ḿ娆戣姳閿燂拷 闂備浇妫勯悧鎾活敁閿燂拷 濠殿喗蓱閸ㄨ泛螞閿燂拷 闂佸搫鍊稿ú銈団偓纰夋嫹 闂佹垝鐒﹂妵鐐电礊閿燂拷 婵°倗濮撮張顒勫蓟閿燂拷 闂佹悶鍎插畷妯侯潩閿燂拷 婵$偛顑囬崰鎾诲几閿燂拷 闂佽偐鐡旈崹铏櫠閿燂拷 闂佽偐鐡旈崹顖滅箔閿燂拷 闂佽偐鐡旈崹鎵博閿燂拷 闂佸搫鍊规刊钘夌暤閿燂拷 闂佺懓鍚嬬划搴ㄥ磼閿燂拷 闂佸吋妲掓ご鎼佸几閿燂拷
当前位置: 首页 > 期刊 > 《英国医生杂志》 > 2005年第17期 > 正文
编号:11385999
Making decisions about mammography
http://www.100md.com 《英国医生杂志》

     Estimates of risks and benefits, should be set out in a straightforward way for patients

    Proponents of breast cancer screening make powerful claims for its role in reducing mortality.1 The evidence is, however, disputed.2 Critics argue that the presentation of information about the benefits of screening in terms of the relative reduction in the risk of dying from breast cancer is misleading and that the absolute reduction in overall mortality should be used.3 Another criticism is that women are given insufficient information about possible harmful consequences.4 In this issue, Barratt et al present a balance sheet of risks and benefits to help patients make informed choices about screening (p 936).5

    The figures on the balance sheet are generated by using a mathematical technique known as Markov modelling. A disease is represented as a process with several states (for example, healthy, diagnosed, treated) and the probabilities of possible transitions between them. The model employed by Barratt et al uses statistics from BreastScreen Australia and the Australian Bureau of Statistics to determine the proportion of women who receive interventions and estimates of mortality from breast cancer and from other causes. Using data from research trials, other models, and systematic reviews, the authors show how the probability of each outcome is affected by participating in screening. The reduction in mortality is set against the increased likelihood of intervention.

    The figures generated may prove controversial. For benefits, Barratt et al estimate that biennial screening from age 60-70 cuts breast cancer deaths from 8.0 per 1000 to 5.0/1000 over this period. If a woman who was screened throughout her 50s and 60s continues to be screened after the age of 70, her risk of dying of breast cancer by age 80, according to the model, is cut from 8.3/1000 to 6.0/1000. These figures are in line with other estimates.6 The surprise, perhaps, is that the achieved gain, certainly in this age group, corresponds to small reductions in overall mortality: from 75.5/1000 to 75/1000 in women aged 60-70 and from 205.6/1000 to 204.1/1000 in 70-80 year olds.

    The principal possible negative outcome of screening is over-diagnosis—the possibility that a woman might undergo unpleasant treatment without improving mortality or quality of life. Inevitably screening will reveal some cancers that would otherwise have gone undetected, not just for a few years but for the rest of a patient's life. The model predicts that, in the 60-70 age range for example, 24.4 cancers would be detected per 1000 women who decline screening, compared with 38.0/1000 in the screened group. Some of the 13.6 extra cancers in the screening group will be over-diagnosis. The balance sheet metaphor implies that all these extra diagnoses are in some sense the cost that is to be set against the benefit of improved mortality. However, as Barratt et al make clear, a percentage of the extra diagnoses will correspond to the earlier detection of cancers that would otherwise figure in the mortality statistics for the 70-80 age group. The question is how many? Barratt et al report that estimates of over-diagnosis vary from 2% to 30% for invasive cancer. The importance of a diagnosis of non-invasive disease is probably even less certain.

    In the light of these uncertainties one would want to test the predictions of the model. Martin et al developed a decision aid for a different application, using similar modelling techniques but very different data and with the aim of advising patients on the impact that smoking cessation could have on their life expectancy.7 The tool was subjected to a particularly stringent validation process, comparing its predictions with actual outcomes for a cohort of patients who had been followed for 30 years. Given the pace of change in the detection and treatment of breast cancer, identifying an appropriate cohort for a comparable test of the tool described by Barratt et al might be difficult and proving the model's predictions accurate perhaps impossible. Arguably using best available estimates of risks and benefits, set out in the most straightforward way, could help patients make informed choices. This is especially true for women older than 70 who, in Australia and in the United Kingdom, have to make a conscious decision if they want to continue to be screened.

    This tool is one of a growing number designed to help clinicians work with patients to choose a course of action, which reflects an individual's preferences and is based on individualised estimates of risk. As our understanding of the risk factors for diseases improves the scope for such tools will extend. A systematic review found such tools to be effective in engaging patients but that evidence of their impact on decisions is variable suggesting that more research into their design and use is required.8

    Paul Taylor, senior lecturer

    Centre for Health Informatics and Multiprofessional Education, University College London, London N19 5LW (p.taylor@chime.ucl.ac.uk)

    Papers p 936

    Competing interests: None declared.

    References

    Patnick J, ed. Changing lives: NHS breast screening programme annual review 2004. NHS Breast Screening Programme 2004. www.cancerscreening.nhs.uk/breastscreen/publications/nhsbsp-annualreview2004.pdf (accessed 15 Apr 2005).

    Gotzsche PC, Olsen O. Is screening for breast cancer with mammography justifiable? Lancet 2000;355: 129-34.

    Gigerenzer G, Edwards A. Simple tools for understanding risks: from innumeracy to insight. BMJ 2003;327: 741-4.

    J?rgensen KJ, G?tzsche PC. Presentation on websites of possible benefits and harms from screening for breast cancer: cross sectional study. BMJ 2004;328: 148.

    Barratt A, Howard K, Irwig L, Salkeld G, Houssami N. Model of outcomes of screening mammography: information to support informed choices. BMJ 2005;330: 936-8.

    Olsen AH, Njor SH, Vejborg I, Schwartz W, Dalgaard P, Jensen MB, et al. Breast cancer mortality in Copenhagen after introduction of mammography screening: cohort study. BMJ 2005;330: 220.

    Martin C, Vanderpump M, French J. Description and validation of a Markov model of survival for individuals free of cardiovascular disease that uses Framingham risk factors. BMC Med Inform Decis Mak 2004;4: 6.

    O'Connor AM, Rostom A, Fiset V, Tetroe J, Entwistle V, Llewellyn-Thomas H, et al. Decision aids for patients facing health treatment or screening decisions: systematic review. BMJ 1999;319: 731-4.
    婵烇絽娲犻崜婵囧閸涱喚顩烽柛娑卞墰鏉╂棃鏌涘▎蹇撯偓浠嬪焵椤掆偓閸犳稓妲愬┑鍥┾枖鐎广儱妫涢埀顒夊灦楠炲骞囬鍛簥婵炶揪绲惧ú鎴犵不閿濆拋鍤堝Δ锔筋儥閸炴挳鏌曢崱鏇犲妽閻㈩垰缍婇幊锟犲箛椤撶偟浠愰梺鍦瑰ú銈囨閳哄懎违闁稿本绋掗悗顔剧磼閺冨倸啸濠⒀勵殜瀵爼宕橀妸褎鍓戞繛瀛樼矊妤犲摜鏁锔藉婵$偛澧界粙濠囨煛婢跺﹤鏆曟慨鐟邦樀閺佸秴鐣濋崘顭戜户闂佽鍠撻崝蹇涱敇缂佹ḿ鈻旈柣鎴烇供閸斿啴鏌¢崒姘煑缂佹顦遍埀顒冾潐缁繘锝為敃鍌氱哗閻犻缚娅g粔鍨€掑顓犫槈闁靛洤娲ㄩ埀顒傤攰濞夋盯宕㈤妶鍥ㄥ鐟滅増甯楅~澶愭偣閸ワ妇涓茬紒杈ㄧ箘閹风娀鎮滈挊澶夌病婵炲濮鹃崺鏍垂閵娾晜鍋ㄥù锝呭暟濡牓姊洪锝嗙殤闁绘搫绻濋獮瀣箣濠婂嫮鎷ㄩ梺鎸庣☉閺堫剟宕瑰⿰鍕浄妞ゆ帊鐒﹂弳顏堟煕閹哄秴宓嗛柍褜鍓氬銊╂偂閿熺姴瑙﹂幖鎼灣缁€澶娒归崗鍏肩殤闁绘繃鐩畷锟犲礃閼碱剚顔戦梺璇″枔閸斿骸鈻撻幋鐐村婵犲﹤鍟幆鍌毭归悩鎻掝劉婵犫偓閹殿喚纾鹃柟瀵稿Х瑜版煡鏌涢幒鏂库枅婵炲懎閰f俊鎾晸閿燂拷

   閻庣敻鍋婇崰鏇熺┍婵犲洤妫橀柛銉㈡櫇瑜帮拷  闂佺ǹ绻楀▍鏇㈠极閻愮儤鍎岄柣鎰靛墮椤庯拷  闁荤姴娲ょ€氼垶顢欓幋锕€绀勯柣妯诲絻缂嶏拷  闂佺懓鍚嬬划搴ㄥ磼閵娾晛鍗抽柡澶嬪焾濡拷   闂佽浜介崝宀勬偤閵娧呯<婵炲棙鍔栫粻娆撴煕濞嗘瑦瀚�   闂佸憡姊绘慨鎾矗閸℃稑缁╅柟顖滃椤ワ拷